menu Contact Us Dizzy Patients Health Care Providers Dizzy Culture Research Site DVD BPPV DVD Tai Chi DVD Understanding Dizziness Acknowledgements Disclaimer Quoting

Head Shaking Nystagmus

Timothy C. Hain, M.D. Marcello Cherchi, M.D., Ph.D.

Page last modified: January 30, 2014

hsn method trace
Method of inducing HSN (also see text) Head-Shaking Nystagmus -- position trace. The head is shaken as is described in the text, after which a nystagmus ensues, typically for about 15 seconds, with a peak velocity of about 15 deg/sec. (Wei et al, 1989)

Head-shaking nystagmus (HSN) is a jerk nystagmus that may follow a prolonged sinusoidal head oscillation.  HSN was first described in 1907 by Robert Bárány (1907). Moritz called it "kopfschutteinnystagmus" (1951). Subsequent studies (Borries 1923; Klestadt 1936; Vogel 1929) described different techniques of eliciting the nystagmus and advanced hypotheses regarding its pathophysiology (Fetter et al. 1990; Hain and Spindler 1993; Halmagyi and Curthoys 1988; Katsarkas et al. 2000; Minagar et al. 2001; Perez et al. 2004)

A video of Head-shaking nystagmus courtesy of Dr. Dario Yacovino (Argentina).

HSN is most commonly elicited in the following way: 

The patient is positioned upright and instrumented so that fixation is removed but horizontal and vertical eye movements can be observed.   The test is best performed using video Frenzel goggles (see figure above).    The test cannot be done without a method of eliminating fixation.

Eye movements are observed in darkness for 10 seconds to obtain a baseline.  Next, the examiner grasps the patient’s head and moves it briskly back and forth in the yaw plane (around the vertical axis) , aiming for a frequency of about 2 Hz and a displacement of the head of approximately 30 degrees to either side.  Ideally, the head should be pitched about 20 degrees downward with respect to vertical so that the axis of rotation is close to being parallel to the axes of the lateral canals, but practically the results of the test are not sensitive to this procedure.  The head-shaking is continued for 20 cycles and then abruptly stopped. 

In normal subjects or persons with symmetrical vestibular loss (such as bilateral vestibular loss), no nystagmus is expected.  In persons with a dynamic imbalance between the ears (such as due to unilateral vestibular neuritis or an acoustic neuroma), a nystagmus is often seen (usually beating towards the “better” ear (Hain et al. 1987; Katsarkas et al. 2000)) which decays over about 30 seconds.  This is referred to as the first phase of nystagmus, because in some cases it is followed by a second phase of nystagmus that is weaker, decays more slowly, and is directed towards the “bad” ear. The main value of seeing a secondary phase is that one can clearly identify the primary phase (sometimes the primary nystagmus is very short). Rarely, horizontal head-shaking produces a non-horizontal nystagmus, such as vertical nystagmus (Wu et al. 2005) or torsional nystagmus (Califano et al. 2001). This is called a “perverted” head-shaking nystagmus. See the "central" section below for more.

Detailed Methods of eliciting head-shaking nystagmus

There are three main variants: Horizontal head-shaking -- rotation of the head in the horizontal plane, vertical head-shaking -- rotation in the vertical plane, and circular head-shaking -- rotation of the head so that the nose traces out a circle in the coronal plane. The usual duration is 20 cycles, aiming for a horizontal or vertical movement of about 30-45 degrees, and a frequency of roughly one cycle/sec.   Some studies have also explored head-shaking with the body in other positions than upright (Kamei et al. 1997; Palla et al. 2005).  Head-shaking using these other axes of rotation does not have a well established clinical indication.

Horizontal HSN

Horizontal head-shaking is generally performed by the examiner who moves the patient's head back and forth at about a frequency of 2 hz, for 20 cycles. The usual excursion is +-30 degrees, as tolerated. Horizontal HSN is often encountered in persons with unilateral vestibular lesions (e.g. Hain et al, 1987). HSN is not very specific or sensitive to caloric weakness (Wei et al, 1989).

Vertical HSN

Vertical head-shaking is perfomed similarly in the vertical plane. Vertical HSN is less useful than horizontal. Its main utility is when a horizontal or torsional nystagmus follows vertical head-shaking, which either reflects a "latent" nystagmus, or a central cross-coupling (Hain et al, 1993).

Circular head-shaking nystagmus.

Circular head-shaking, first described in a book chapter by Hain and Spindler (1993), is performed by moving the head so that the nose follows the outline of a circle, in front of the patient's head. If this is done for 10 cycles fairly briskly, on stopping a very brisk rotatory nystagmus can be seen with frenzel goggles or any other method of observing the eyes. Circular HSN is mainly a torsional eye movement, which is commonly found in normal subjects and reflects a post-rotatory nystagmus (Hain et al, 1993; Haslwanter and Minor, 1999).

Unlike the horizontal and vertical HSN discussed above, this so-called "circular HSN" occurs quite reproducably in normal individuals. It can be shown (with some effort) that circular-HSN is a consequence of geometry. In this type of movement, the head is in-effect rotating about the front-back axis of the head (without getting twisted off !). When the head-stops, what ensues is simply a post-rotatory nystagmus.Loss of circular HSN is probably abnormal and expected in persons with bilateral vestibular loss.

Method of Circular Head Shaking Recording of Circular Head Shaking Nystagmus

Patterns of HSN

Head-shaking nystagmus in the horizontal and vertical planes is abnormal. A well adjusted vestibular system doesn't produce head-shaking nystagmus, at least not for more than a second or so, because it reflects a unidirectional output for a sinusoidal input. This is abnormal and either means that the vestibular-ocular motor system is rectifying the input (clipping it in one direction), or that the system is being perturbed by the input. One would expect HSN to beat away from the "bad" side. One would also expect HSN to be followed by a secondary or "reversal" phase, caused by adaptation of the vestibular system. This is indeed often the case.

Unfortunately, HSN is not 100% reliable, being neither always appropriately directed nor present. The details of this are reviewed by Hain/Spindler (1993). Recent papers have suggested that HSN has insufficient sensitivity to be useful as a clinical test (Humphriss et al, 2003).

Mechanism of HSN

HSN is messy ! Several classes of pathophysiological mechanisms have been proposed as etiologies of HSN.  These are discussed in detail by Hain and Spindler (Hain and Spindler 1993), who point out that probably more than one mechanism is involved since HSN can have central or peripheral etiologies.  The possible mechanisms are listed in the following table.

Table 1: Hypothesized mechanisms underlying head-shaking nystagmus

The first class of hypothesized pathophysiological mechanisms involves the existence of a tone asymmetry that predisposes to a nystagmus which is usually absent due to compensatory mechanisms, but which is manifest when the compensatory mechanisms are disrupted (e.g., by head-shaking). 

In the old German literature, this was called "latent nystagmus". Note that the term “latent nystagmus” is also more commonly used to denote a variant of congenital nystagmus. From a systems analysis perspective, this also derives from nonlinearity, but it need not necessarily scale with the size of the input

The second class of mechanisms involves asymmetries in vestibular gain due to a variety of factors, such as lesions to the vestibular nerve or root entry zone, loss of vestibular hair cells, asymmetrical cervical afferent input, or asymmetrical central gain. 

Asymmetry in the peripheral vestibular input leads to nystagmus by virtue of Ewald’s second law, which in its specific form states that ampullopetal endolymph flow in the horizontal canal causes a greater response than ampullofugal endolymph flow (Baloh and Honrubia 2001; Ewald 1892), and which in its general form states that excitation is a relatively better vestibular stimulus than is inhibition (Leigh and Zee 2006).  Ewald’s second law is thought to be due to the inability of inhibitory stimuli to decrease vestibular nerve firing rates to less than zero (Baloh et al. 1977; Hain and Spindler 1993).

Asymmetrical neck input or neck modulation is a very reasonable source of HSN that so far, has gone unexplored.

The third class of mechanisms involves asymmetries in timing. 

Hain and Spindler ( 1993) conceptualized this class of mechanisms as deriving from errors in central head movement response storage.  Storage mechanisms, sometimes called “leaky integrators,” are commonly encountered in central nervous system circuitry as they provide low-pass filtering and averaging.  Since responses are stored in three places in the vestibular system (the semicircular canals, the central velocity storage mechanism, and adaptation circuitry), there are also three corresponding potential sources of asymmetry in timing.  First, response storage asymmetry in the semicircular canals could result, for example, from asymmetrical endolymph viscosity between the ears, leading to a timing mismatch and filtering across the two ears.  Second, there can be asymmetry in the central velocity storage mechanism.  The central velocity storage mechanism prolongs the raw vestibular signal (Leigh and Zee 2006); its neuroanatomical substrate is believed to be the medial vestibular nuclei of Schwalbe and their connections (Baloh and Honrubia 2001; Leigh and Zee 2006).  Third, there can be asymmetry in the compensatory changes involved in central and/or peripheral adaptation (Leigh and Zee 2006).

The fourth class of mechanisms involves simple mechanical causes, such as improper technique of eliciting the HSN. 

When head movement is not confined to a single plane, as in the “circular trajectory” as previously described (Hain and Spindler, 1993; Haslwanter and Minor 1999),  there is an effective rotation about an axis of the head that induces a per-rotatory or post-rotatory nystagmus.  Thus, one must be careful to maintain a constant and consistent axis when doing head-shaking to avoid extraneous nystagmus.  Because normal subjects should exhibit post-rotatory predominantly torsional nystagmus after “circular trajectory” head shaking, its absence may be a clue to the presence of bilateral vestibular hypofunction.

Overall, there are numerous potential mechanisms for HSN.  This logic would suggest that it should be sensitive but not specific.

Models of Head Shaking Nystagmus

We have proposed several explicit mechanisms for simulating HSN, based on the idea that HSN is driven by asymmetries in vestibualar gain.   The asymmetry in vestibular gain was first observed by Ewald (Ewald 1892), and is referred to as Ewald’s second law. In its specific form it states that ampullopetal endolymph flow in the horizontal canal causes a greater response than ampullofugal endolymph flow (Ewald 1892; Baloh and Honrubia 2001). In its general form it states that excitation is a relatively better vestibular stimulus than is inhibition (Leigh and Zee 2006).  Ewald’s second law is thought to be due to the inability of inhibitory stimuli to decrease vestibular nerve firing rates to less than zero (Baloh, Honrubia et al. 1977; Hain and Spindler 1993). This phenomenon may be caused by an ensemble of vestibular neurons, all having somewhat different tonic rates, gradually being driven into inhibitory cutoff.

Bilateral mathematical model of vestibular processing from Hain (Hain, Fetter et al. 1987). This early model does not include acceleration in the canal transfer function, but otherwise contains the general features described below.

We can take these ideas and convert them into a more quantitative form. We will not do this formally, but just try to get the general concepts converted into a mathematical form. First, firing rate in the vestibular nerve is mainly proportional to head velocity, but there is also a component due to head acceleration. So one can (approximately) say that:

Firing rate = K0+Kv*head-velocity

(Vestibular physiologists would shudder at this oversimplification). Continuing, because firing rate has a lower limit of 0 spikes/second, there is a saturation of the overall response for the nerve. In the diagram above, this is the little box with the curved line (nonlinearity) inside of it.

Net firing rate = saturation*Firing rate

The vestibular system is hooked up in "push-pull" so that the firing rate for one side is subtracted from the other. In other words:

central firing rate = right-left.

This is the circle with the +- in the center. When both vestibular nerves are working, net firing rate (right-left) shows a saturation in both direction.

When just one vestibular nerve is working, there is a strong response for rotation in that excites the remaining nerve (i.e. contralateral to the lesion for the lateral canal), and a weaker response for rotation for rotation towards the lesion.

The cross-connected boxes that implement velocity storage. This stores the asymmetry and explains the decay when the eyes stop.

This (fairly) simple theory explains HSN in quantative terms. In other words, it is feasible. This is not the same as being established to be true.

Another model -- using the latent nystagmus idea:

As mentioned in the mechanistic discussion, the old German literature suggested that HSN was due to a nystagmus that was revealed by head shaking. The unpublished model below implements this idea, showing that it is feasible (though not necessarily correct).

Model of HSN caused by a perturbation (Hain and Spindler, 1993) Simulation

These two figures are suggestions for a central mechanism for HSN. Head-shaking opens a switch in the velocity storage mechanism, creating a transient central nystagmus.

Models of other mechanisms

There are several other quite reasonable explanations for HSN, as outlined in the table above. For example, timing asymmetries or cervical nystagmus. It would be very easy to implement a timing asymmetry model as a variant of the bilateral model above. We hope that these ideas are eventually investigated, and modeled.

Clinical observations concerning HSN

Testing for HSN is useful in detecting vestibular asymmetry of peripheral or central origin (Leigh and Zee 2006).

Unilateral vestibular loss

HSN in acoustic neuroma HSN in patient with acoustic neuroma. A (long) video of HSN in a patient with unilateral loss is here.

acoustic neuroma 2

HSN in four patients with acoustic neuroma. Horizontal head shaking elicits an initial slow-phase velocity beating away from the paretic side. Vertical head-shaking elicits an initial slow-phase velocity towards the paretic side.

Head-shaking nystagmus is routinely found in persons with acoustic neuroma.  The nystagmus induced by head-shaking nearly always exhibits a slow phase directed towards the abnormal side (Baloh and Honrubia 2001; Hain et al. 1987).  In our clinical material we have observed that while the direction of nystagmus is consistent, the amplitude of the nystagmus does not scale with the severity of vestibular loss.

Other vestibular pathologies

Head-shaking nystagmus is also reported in similar peripheral pathologies, such as vestibular neuritis (Choi et al. 2007b) and following vestibular nerve section.  Head-shaking nystagmus has also been reported in association with peripheral pathologies such as Ménière’s disease (Asawavichiangianda et al. 1999; Kamei and Iizuka 1999) and benign paroxysmal positional vertigo (Asawavichiangianda et al. 1999) in which the mechanism is not clear.  In Ménière’s disease, we have observed ourselves that HSN direction is inconsistent.  We do not recommend using HSN for the diagnosis of either of these conditions.

Since head-shaking nystagmus in persons with peripheral vestibular lesions is thought to reflect a combination of asymmetrical input and central velocity storage (Hain et al. 1987), one would expect that HSN would be largest in persons who have moderate vestibular lesions, because in there is asymmetrical input but intact central velocity storage.  In complete unilateral loss, central velocity storage is lost, and theory would predict a very brief or no HSN.

In general, HSN should result from any vestibular nonlinearity , whether it be peripheral or central, and could also result from interactions from neck afferents which are, of course, also stimulated by head-shaking.

In theory, for HSN to be generated, the nonlinearity must also be accompanied by some dynamics to average and retain the clipped input. In the past we have proposed that the velocity storage mechanism performs this function.

Central vestibular problems

Head-shaking nystagmus can also result from central lesions such as lateral medullary infarction (Choi et al. 2007c).  When found in association with central lesions, the direction of nystagmus is non-localizing (Baloh and Honrubia 2001; Califano et al. 1999). 

Perverted head-shaking nystagmus (i.e., nystagmus in a plane other than that being stimulated) can result from central lesions, such as in the cerebellum (Kim et al. 2005; Walker and Zee 1999a) or medulla (Minagar et al. 2001). In general, the mechanism for perverted HSN is thought to be either a directional error in the input circuitry, or a directional error in the central velocity storage mechanism that perseverates vestibular input.

Perverted HSN has been reported in MSA (multiple system atrophy) by Lee et al (2009). We are very dubious about the value of this observation and suspect that the methods of detecting pHSN were simply overly sensitive. It also makes little sense that there should be HSN at all in MSA, as this is not an inner ear disorder. Similar findings by this group in other disorders, such as cerebellar infarction (Huh and Kim, 2011), and pontine infarction (Kim and Lee, 2011) are also (in our opinion) very suspicious, due to the same logic.

In our clinical practice in Chicago, the type of pHSN that we mainly see is weak downbeating nystagmus in persons with migraine associated vertigo. We would also generally think unremarkable perverted nystagmus in any patient with a substantial cerebellar lesionbut we would not go so far to think of this as a sensitive clinical sign.

Although HSN can occur with either peripheral or central lesions, overall it is seen far more commonly in peripheral lesions (Vicini et al. 1989).  Presumably this is due to the predominance of peripheral over central lesions in persons with dizziness.

Central HSN

Situations where it seems likely that HSN is simply a result of perturbation are generally central ones. In the figure above entitled "lateral medullary syndrome", an extremely powerful HSN is produced, which is inconsistent with the amount of nystagmus produced in complete peripheral vestibular lesions. This could in theory be either due to a more drastic central nonlinearity in this patient with a vestibular nucleus infarct, or from perturbation. In the figure above entitled "cerebellar degeneration" is an example of a strong head-shaking nystagmus is given where the reversal phase is larger than the primary phase.

Sensitivity of HSN to disease

Studies conducted regarding the sensitivity of HSN in undifferentiated peripheral vestibular lesions (Asawavichiangianda et al. 1999; Burgio et al. 1991; Guidetti et al. 2006; Humphriss et al. 2003; Iwasaki et al. 2004; Jacobson et al. 1990; Kamei et al. 1964; Nuti and Mandala 2005; Takahashi et al. 1990; Tseng and Chao 1997; Wei et al. 1989) report sensitivities ranging from 20.7% to 95%. 

The range is so broad because the studies use different inclusion and exclusion criteria; they compare the results against a variety of other tests; and they use different ENG criteria for canal paresis.  Therefore it is difficult to formulate generalizations concerning the sensitivity of this test for peripheral lesions.  As mentioned above, one would expect that HSN would not be 100% sensitive to peripheral vestibular lesions, because as the degree of vestibular asymmetry increases, central velocity storage decreases.  As HSN probably requires both asymmetry and storage, it should be absent or brief in persons with complete vestibular loss.

Specificity of HSN to disease

Head-shaking nystagmus is specific for vestibular asymmetry if it is positive in persons with vestibular asymmetry and negative in other conditions in which vestibular function is normal.  Hain and Spindler (1993) outlined many non-vestibular conditions in which HSN might be generated.

Several studies have evaluated the specificity of head-shaking nystagmus in a variety of conditions (Burgio et al. 1991; Guidetti et al. 2006; Guidetti et al. 2000; Humphriss et al. 2003; Iwasaki et al. 2004; Jacobson et al. 1990).  Although the specificity of HSN in these studies ranges from 65% to 98.8%, the lesion localization and differences in criteria make generalizations concerning the specificity of this test tenuous.

Head-shaking nystagmus that is present on initial examination may not persist—in other words, it may not be durable.  Guidetti et al. (Guidetti et al. 2002) examined patients with a variety of vestibular pathologies and found that after six months of pharmacological treatment or physical therapy or both, 39% of patients no longer exhibited HSN.

Overall, Head-shaking nystagmus is moderately sensitive to unilateral peripheral vestibular lesions, although the exact sensitivity and specificity of HSN for particular conditions are unknown (Hain and Spindler 1993).  From consideration of its mechanism of action for vestibular asymmetry, it should be moderately sensitive but nonspecific.

Reliability of HSN

HSN is somewhat erratic. As illustrated by the figure below, in patients with complete unilateral loss, while the direction is consistent, the size and duration varies.

HSN erratic

 

Falls and Head-shaking nystagmus

Head-shaking nystagmus is an indication of asymmetrical vestibular function. Kristindottir et al (2000) rcently reported that there is a higher frequency of HSN in hip fracture subjects than halthy subjects. Postural stability is impaired when headshaking is performed in normal persons as well as in persons with vestibular problems (Paloski et al, 1995).

References:

 



Copyright October 6, 2013 , Timothy C. Hain, M.D. All rights reserved. Last saved on October 6, 2013