menu Contact Us Dizzy Patients Health Care Providers Dizzy Culture Research Site DVD BPPV DVD Tai Chi DVD Understanding Dizziness Acknowledgements Disclaimer Quoting

Mal de Debarquement Syndrome (MDD or MdDS)

Timothy C. Hain, MD Page last modified: June 15, 2016


Mal de Debarquement or "MdDS" (or MDD) is a type of vertigo and imbalance that occurs after getting off of a boat. The usual situation is that of a middle aged woman who has gone on a cruise. We use the abbreviation "MdDS" because it is currently favored over the simpler MDD. It has been pointed out that "MDD" can be confused with other disorders. MdDS has attracted more interest recently, and there are several recent reviews -- Saha et al (2014), and Van Ombergen et al (2015).

To our knowledge, the first reference to the syndrome was made by Erasmus Darwin, in 1796. He wrote:

"Those, who have been upon the water in a boat or ship so long, that they have acquired the necessary habits of motion upon that unstable element, at their return on land frequently think in their reveries, or between sleeping and waking, that they observe the room, they sit in, or some of its furniture, to librate like the motion of the vessel. This I have experienced myself, and have been told, that after long voyages, it is some time before these ideas entirely vanish. The same is observable in a less degree after having travelled some days in a stage coach, and particularly when we lie down in bed, and compose ourselves to sleep; in this case it is observable, that the rattling noise of the coach, as well as the undulatory motion, haunts us. " (Darwin, 1796).

Some Cases:

A 50-year-old woman went on her first ocean cruise. She had some motion sickness on the cruise, which responded to transdermal scopolamine. Immediately after returning from the cruise and getting onto solid ground, she developed imbalance and a rocking sensation, accompanied by fatigue and difficulty concentrating. This persisted for months. She felt better however when she was driving. Her description was “Imagine feeling like you are on rough seas 24 hours a day, 7 days a week.”

A related case ? An avid cross country skier noted that after she would come home from a day of skiing, she developed a sensation as if she was still skiing until she went to sleep. She was always fine in the morning. Many patients with MdDS or rocking symptoms say they have "motion memory" meaning that they feel as if they are moving after driving, or sometimes even just riding on an elevator.

General characteristics of MDD

Table1: Characteristics of MDD

Authors

Subjects

Age

Females

Males

Median Duration (mo)

Cha et al, 2008

64

39

48

16

Variable

Hain et al, 1999

27

49.3 (38-76)

26

1

3.5 years (6 mo to 10 years)

Brown and Baloh, 1987

6

50.8(33-71)

5

1

4 mo

Murphy, 1993

4

40(36-48)

4

0

1-12 mo

Mair, 1996

10

37.5(15-55)

9

0

1-6 mo

Total or (average)

111

(43.3)

82 (85%)

14 (14.5%)

months to years

 

Table 1 summarizes the available literature about MDD. Ombergen et al (2015) discovered far more patients than this, and the interested reader is referred to their systemic review. It is a disorder that mainly affects women (85%) in their mid 40's. Except in California where a large number of men are affected, (the site of Dr. Cha's study), almost all individuals with MdDS are women. They typically have gone on a 7-day cruise. After getting off the boat, or "debarking" (debarquement), they immediately develop a rocking sensation, as if they are still on the boat.

Here we define MdDS as rocking lasting for at least a month. This leaves the land-sickness group out. This definition was also used by the majority of studies reviewed by Ombergen et al (2015).

mdds age
Age of onset of patients in Chicago Dizziness and Hearing database, as of 2015.

The graph above is drawn on a much larger and more representative sample of patients than from our study of 1999, again mainly women (153/181 -- 85%), it shows that the distribution mainly includes females between the ages of 30-60.

How common is Mal de Debarquement ?

As of 2015, we had encountered 181 MdDS patients in our dizzy clinic database out of an "n" of about 18,000 patients with assigned diagnoses. This suggests that only roughly 1/100 dizzy patients have MDD. As about 5% of the population develops "vestibular" dizziness per year, if we assume that our clinic sees MdDS as a representative proportion of all dizzy patients, a very very rough estimate as to the prevalence of MdDS is 0.05% (this is the same as 0.5/1000 persons). Or to put this into real-world terms, assuming 300 million persons in the United States, a 0.5 persons/1000 people prevalence works out to a total of 150,000 persons with MdDS in the US. Compared to land sickness (which happens in roughly 50% of persons who go on boats), this is an unusual condition, and far less common than other dizzy conditions, say vestibular migraine (1% of population -- 3 million) or Meniere's disease (2/1000 people, 600,000 total in US or 0.2%). Note that this estimate could easily be wrong (probably too much) by an order of magnitude.

 

Duration of MDD

The figure above, from our 1999 paper, illustrates "worst case" durations -- it does not reflect the usual duration of symptoms. This is because the study group from our 1999 paper were persons with very severe MDD. A prospective study would be needed to answer this question properly.

What is and isn't MdDS ? Motion sickness and Land-sickness is not MdDS.

Table 2: Features Distinguishing MdDS from Land-sickness (LDS)

 

MdDS

Land sickness

Duration

1 or more months

2 days maximum

Gender

About 90% female

Equal distribution

Motion-sick on boat

No

Yes

Relieved by driving

Yes

No

There are several variants of motion induced sickness that are not MdDS . Table 2 lists the features that distinguish MDD from simple land-sickness. Land-sickness (LDS) is common, and between 41% and 73% of persons disembarking from seagoing voyages experience a brief unsteadiness syndrome (Gordon, Spitzer et al. 1995; Cohen 1996; Gordon, Shupak et al. 2000). Common LDS typically persists for 2 days or less. Persons with LDS are also likely to have sea-sickness, (Gordon, Spitzer et al. 1995) while persons with MdDS generally are untroubled by sea-sickness. Males and females do not appear differ significantly in the incidence, intensity, or duration of land-sickness symptoms. (Cohen 1996). LDS, confusingly, is also termed "mal de debarquement" by some. Table 1 does not include reports or data concerning subjects who symptoms that last less than 1 month, i.e. potential land-sickness, except for the work of Cha, in whom the duration of patients with "classic" MdDS could not be determined due to study design(Cha, Brodsky et al. 2008).

Ombergen, Rompaey, Maes, Heyning and Wuyts (2015), in a "systemic review", invented another nomenclature -- transient "MdD" symptoms < 48h, persistent MdDS (> 3 days to several years). "Transient MdD", using their nomenclature is equivalent to land-sickness as defined above. There is an undefined group for Ombergen et al (between 2-3 days). From 3 days to several years would be MdDS as we define here, and then from "several years" onward, no name.

MDD also has some similarities to motion sickness (sea sickness, mal de mer). However, MdDS is again easily distinguished by the shorter duration of motion sickness and gender distribution. Persons with MdDS reliably have relief of symptoms when in motion, such as driving a car, but experience recurrence of rocking once motion has stopped (Hain, Hanna et al. 1999; Cha, Brodsky et al. 2008). In motion sickness, many persons find driving very difficult. This is also often true for persons with vestibular disorders.

MdDS also overlaps with a little studied group of patients called "rockers", who develop similar symptoms to MdDS, without a preceding motion exposure. (Cha 2012) Another name for the same group is "non motion triggered MdDS" -- which is of course a contradiction in terms, but it presumably serves to widen the spectrum of "MdDS". Often these patients develop head or trunk rocking, which is called "titubation". In our clinical experience, the age, gender and pattern of medication responsiveness of this group are similar to those of MdDS. Although titubation is associated with cerebellar disturbance, evidence of cerebellar damage is generally not found in "rockers". Cohen et al (2015) stated that their term for "rockers" is "spontaneous MdDS ". They found that these people were more difficult to treat with habituation but still "the cure rate was more than 50%".

We have also encountered a few patients, or "rockers", who developed this symptom after a well defined CNS lesion, generally a small stroke. This is usually in the motion processing area of the brain.

What causes Mal de Debarquement ?

It is the general opinion that MdDS is generally not caused by an injury to the ear or brain. At this writing (2016), the predominant opinion is that MdDS is a variant motion sickness. While this theory doesn't explain why MdDS seems to mainly affect women in their 40-60's it does seem to account for observations of naval personnel who have a similar land-sickness experience.

Some dizziness experts believe that MdDS is caused by a variant of migraine. We do think that migraine increases risk of MdDS. We have seen many patients who developed MdDS when exposed to motion around the time of their period (which is also a high risk time for migraine). This might explain why some cruises are tolerated without distress, and others not.

Other theories about the cause of MdDS it that is caused by inappropriately high weighting of somatosensory input for balance (Naichem et al, 2004). As somatosensory information and vestibular information are both unreliable on a boat, this is a difficult explanation to follow. We think that the internal model theory explanation (which follows) is the better one at this writing (2014).

A recent conjecture is that MdDS is caused by adaptation to roll while rotating. In other words, if one is rocking side-side (roll), and also rotating the head, for long periods of time, one might develop an inappropriate cross-coupling between roll and rotation (Dai et al, 2009; 2014). Our position on this idea is that it could explain brief (2 hour) symptoms after getting off of a boat, and also offers an explicit hypothesis that might be tested formally (i.e. people who do a lot of head rotation on a boat would be more likely to get this than people who sit quietly). However, overall we are dubious that this conjecture is correct. Why would women mainly develop inappropriate cross-coupling ? Why wouldn't people get rid of this by simply walking around with their eyes open ?

Because the condition largely occurs in females, it may also have something to do with sex hormones, such as estrogen or progesterone. In fact, we have noted a pattern that if one asks, it is often the case that the woman who develops MdDS was having a period while on the boat. MdDS could also be genetic, related to two copies of the 'X' chromosome perhaps combined with other susceptibility factors. The "Norwalk" virus is common on cruise ships, and perhaps this syndrome is somehow related to this virus.

It seems unlikely to be a psychological disturbance -- although it is always difficult to entirely exclude psychological problems, the male:female ratio and other aspects of this disorder would make this unlikely.

Moeller and Lempert (2007) recently suggested that MdDS is due to "deafferentation" or panic. We disagree with both of these ideas.

Cha et al (2012) reported changes in brain connectivity in persons with MdDS. This study, however, was done in a powerful MRI scanner, which can cause temporary dizziness by itself. MdDS seems to be associated with changes metabolism in the brain, in circuits related to vision, vestibular processing and emotional reactions. It would be interesting to see how much of this is due to MdDS and how much is secondary to being dizzy. In 2015, in a study published in PLOS-1 (open-access), Cha and Chakrapani reported that " Individuals with MdDS show brain volume differences from healthy controls as well as duration of illness dependent volume changes in (a) visual-vestibular processing areas (IPL, SPL, V3, V5/MT), (b) default mode network structures (cerebellar IX, IPL, ACC), (c) salience network structures (ACC and IFG/AI) (d) somatosensory network structures (postcentral gyrus, MCC, anterior cerebellum, cerebellar lobule VIII), and (e) a structure within the central executive network (DLPFC). " 80% of the 29 subjects in this study were female, almost all were middle aged, and almost all had symptoms for more than 1 month; thus they met the conventional criteria for this MdDS. The authors themselves point out that the brain volume differences might be due to compensatory processes rather than causes of symptoms. They also note that the results were presented with "uncorrected p-values and it can be argued that many of the voxels seen in the contrasts could be seen by chance". Overall, these studies do not seem to have added much to our understanding of MdDS.

There are some reports of MdDS following use or withdrawal from serotonergic medications. The connection here is that serotonin may inhibit glutamate, an excitatory transmitter in the vestibular nucleus (Smith and Darlington, 2010.). This idea also provides an explanation why serotonergic medications may help MdDS (see treatment section).

MdDS and MDD

Note that the MdDS of "Mal de Debarquement" has nothing to do with the other MDD of "Manic Depressive Disorder". The support group for MdDS, being sensitive to this, has suggested that the proper name of the syndrome would best be "MdDS ". Our own feeling is that there are many medical disorders that share initials with other disorders (PAN is an example -- periodic alcohol nystagmus and periodic alternating nystagmus), and one should not get too anxious about the initials

Internal model theory and MdDS

A plausible mechanism for the development of MdDS is that it is due to formation of an inappropriate internal predictive model. We first proposed this idea in 2007 (Hain and Helminski, 2007). Internal models are sophisticated estimators that have been used to explain such difficult observations that one cannot "tickle" oneself (see the work of Wolpert (1995) and others). Examples of internal models are very easy to find in daily life -- suppose you pick up a suitcase, expecting it to be full, but it is empty ! Internal models are sophisticated methods of reacting to events even before they happen ! (Blakemore, 1998)

On a boat, one is faced with a difficult balancing problem, with components of rotation (pitch plane rotation -- about the axis between the ears), and linear movement (surge -- front-back movement of the boat). Both are somewhat predictable as the boat is large and it's interface between it and the ocean constrains it to low frequencies of movement.

Lets take an example -- when the boat pitches (rotates) forward, there is a small amount of pushing the person backward accompanied by a tilt of the visual world as the angle between upright and the boat surface becomes more acute. To stay upright in response to pitch, a person should not activate their ankle muscles much as inertia tends to keep their body upright. Vision is accurate on the deck but inaccurate inside. Thus vision is unreliable. Although there is rotation around the ankle joint, and thus somatosensory input, there should be no "righting" response from the person because the body is upright in space. Visual responses are correct on deck and incorrect inside, and thus a "rule" cannot be made. The rule then for pitch rotation of the boat, one should ignore somatosensory information signaling rotation . Thus for pitch of the boat, a selective "downweighting" of somatosensory information, or both somatosensory and visual information according to context, would be a reasonable adaptation (or rule).

For linear acceleration of the boat under the person, or "surge" as it is called in nautical contexts, inertia attempts to keep the person still in space, but due to shear force at the feet, the person becomes destabilized and rotates at the ankles. Then vision, vestibular and somatosensation senses are activated by the bodily rotation with respect to the boat, and an active response is needed to prevent a fall. Thus for surge of the boat, no relative sensory reweighting would be needed, although increased responses to all types of input might be helpful.

How does the brain figure which rule to apply ? We propose that people develop a predictive model of the boat motion, and use their prediction to select the rule to apply for boat motion (and avoid falling).

Normally, it seems likely that over a few days, people develop an internal model of periodic motion on the boat so that they predict and cancel out input (visual or somatosensory) that is phase-locked to pitch rotation, and enhance responses due to surge that is not. The internal model normally is disposed of once the person returns to terra firma, again over a period of hours to days. Persons with MdDS are unable to dispose of this internal model, which is only useful when they are exposed to periodic motion (such as when driving a car).

We have encountered a few patients whose motion after-effects are specific to motion - -when they cross country ski, they have a motion after-effect of skiing, etc. Of course, this has to be an internal model problem.

The internal model theory explains most of the features of MdDS.

What does the data say about mechanism ?

With respect to the hypothesis that MdDS is caused by reweighting of visual, vestibular or somatosensory input, the data so far is contradictory. Nachum and associates used posturography to study young males aged 18-22 with motion sickness and land-sickness (they considered land-sickness to be equivalent to mal de debarquement in their paper -- see table 2 above). They reported that these young men developed increased reliance on somatosensory input after motion exposure, and reduced weighting of vision and vestibular input (Nachum, Shupak et al. 2004). While the accuracy of visual input depends on whether one is inside the boat or on the deck, semicircular canal input is accurate on boats, and somatosensory input is intermittently accurate. Accordingly, it is difficult to understand a rationale for this adaptation. An intrinsic problem with this study is that the study group were young men with motion sickness and transient land-sickness, not middle aged women with the month-or-greater MdDS syndrome.

Stoffregen et al (2013) also studied a different group than the usual MdDS sufferers -- 40 students of average age 20.68 years, oddly enough, without reporting their gender. Like Naichum et al, they defined MdDS to be landsickness, and thus they were studying something other than MdDS, but calling it MdDS. They defined "Low-MD" as symptoms for 30 minutes or less, and "High-MD" as symptoms for 120 minutes or more. Because this paper's definition of MdDS and subject population is so different than the clinical population in which the medical community diagnoses MDD, they were studying landsickness but they were calling it MDD, and there is little to be gained in considering their work further. It is interesting to note that their paper was published in Plos, an open source journal. While the PLOS journals are very accessible, their review process is quite different than the traditional one that involves experts in the field. In other words, publications in PLOS carries less weight than publication in a standard journal.

Well, at any rate, rather than the somatosensory weighting process suggested by Naichum, a more reasonable possibility is that individuals with MDD may develop an increased reliance on visual and vestibular information (and thus decreased somatosensory weighting). This occurs in normal subjects who are exposed to situations where somatosensory feedback is distorted (Peterka, 2002), and would also be a reasonable adaptation to boat pitch. Either adaptation might result in inaccurate land sensorimotor integration. Nevertheless, neither of these adaptations explain the rocking sensation of MDD or the characteristic improvement on driving a car.

The most recent mechanistic proposition for MDD is that of Dai and associates (2014). They proposed that MDDs was caused by maladaptation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) to roll of the head during rotation, and reported that a 5 day long protocol attempting to readapt the VOR resulted in “substantial recovery on average for approximately 1 year” of 17 of 24 subjects. While these results are encouraging, this theory does not explain why patients with MDD are better while driving. Furthermore, it is difficult to see why ordinary movement through the environment should not recalibrate the VOR over a few days – the usual upper limit for the duration of land-sickness. At the time of this writing in 2015, the roll adaptation theory and treatment protocol needs more study.

Diagnosis of Mal de Debarquement

In our opinion, the diagnosis of MDD is made by a combination of the history (rocking after prolonged exposure to a boat or other source of prolonged motion), improvement with driving, and exclusion of reasonable alternatives. A motion exposure of 2 hours is a bare minimum. The typical duration of exposure is a week. Tests to exclude Menieres disease should be done (i.e. hearing tests) and vestibular neuritis (usually bedside testing is enough), and if there is a history of plane flight, perilymph fistula should also be considered.

Ombergen et al (2015) suggested similar diagnostic criteria:

  1. Chronic rocking dizziness that started after either passive motion or exposure to virtual reality
  2. Symptoms lasting for at least 1 month
  3. Normal inner ear function or unrelated abnormalities on ENG/VNG or audiological testing
  4. Normal structural brain imaging
  5. Symptoms not better accounted for by another diagnosis

Ombergen et al did not include improvement on driving in their criteria, but otherwise these seem quite reasonable.

A typical patient is a woman between 40-60 years of age, who has gone on a cruise, and who is now rocking. Recently, the criterion has been expanded to require getting better on driving. This feature is extremely common in MDD, but rare (but not at all unheard of) in inner ear disorders or Migraine.

Testing:

Cohen et al (2015) state that "although vestibular testing can be of use, in general, MRIs, VEMPS, auditory evoked potentials and multiple blood tests really have nothing to offer diagnostically". That being said, MRI's are commonly done.

GIbbs et al (2010), determined that the Romberg test is not sensitive to "sea legs" (landsickness). This is puzzling given that a prominent complaint is imbalance.

In our practice, we get the following tests, in persons who are not "classic" (i.e. "rockers") -- i.e. not improved by driving and not on a cruise, see table 1:

We have not found that MDD patients exhibit abnormally largevestibular gains or abnormally long time constants. They generally test normally on the rotatory chair, unless they are taking benzodiazepines (which decrease VOR gain and increase VOR time constants).

Treatment for Mal de Debarquement

The usual treatment strategy for MDD is to attempt to make the patient comfortable, while waiting for the MDD to end by itself (typically within 6 months, see table 1). After MDD has started, most medications that work for other forms of dizziness or motion sickness are ineffective. Specifically, antivert (bonine, meclizine), dramamine, and scopolamine seem to be of little use. The author has tried out many other medications, and has also not found response to more unusual agents for dizziness such as betahistine, baclofen, or verapamil.

Conventional vestibular suppressants that affect anticholinergic pathways such as meclizine and transdermal scopolamine are not helpful in MDD. (Hain, Hanna et al. 1999) Benzodiazepines, such as clonazepam, are of the most benefit. (Hain, Hanna et al. 1999; Cha 2012), and SSRI type antidepressants are also suggested as being potentially helpful (Cha 2012). There are also anecdotal reports of good responses to gabapentin, amitriptyline, and venlafaxine. Others have reported that nortriptyline is helpful. All of these are medications that are also helpful in migraine.

We do not see that MdDS should generally be a reason to pull someone's drivers license, as generally MdDS symptoms remit entirely while driving.

Medications to stop and procedures to consider stopping.

New treatment reported using optokinetic stimulation.

Dai et al (2014) reported successful treatment of MDD in about 60% of cases using a procedure involving optokinetic visual stimulation and tilting of the head about the front-back axis (roll). The procedures involves multiple short sessions over a week. The study was uncontrolled. According to Cohen et al (2015), more than 100 patients have been treated wit this protocol, and the "cure rate" is similar to the original report (about 60%). This protocol and result has been replicated in a few patients at Chicago Dizziness and Hearing. As noted above, we are dubious that roll adaptation explains MDDs, and for this reason we are also dubious about the rationale for this treatment. Still, it seems harmless and given that it seems successful both in New York as well as in our clinic, we think it reasonable to try in MDDs that has not resolved with time. We are presently treating patients in our dizzy practice in Chicago.

Recent reports by Dai and colleagues suggest that the optokinetic treatment is far more successful in the classic motion triggered "MdDS", than the group variously called "rockers", or non-motion triggered MdDS (which is an odd construction). This suggests that they are different conditions, that have been wrongly lumped together.

New treatment reported using TMS.

Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was reported by Cha to be associated with “short-term symptom improvement”, in a pilot study of Cha in 2013 as well as helpful in 5 of 10 subjects in more recent studies of Guofa et al (2015) and Pearce (2015). More study is needed of this treatment modality for MDD. In TMS, generally any changes are temporary. For example, although TMS can be used to treat depression, one needs to do it over and over again every week. A treatment protocol that accomplishes long term improvement in this disorder that lasts months to years is crucial. See our "exciting stuff" page concerning our attempt to find a provider for TMS for MDDs patients in Chicago.

Prevention of MDD

Medications taken prior and during boat travel might prevent development of MDD.

Physical Therapy for MDD

Physical therapy: The evidence for a positive role for physical therapy in MDD is somewhere between nonexistent and weak (Hain and Helminski, 2007). In our original study, 10/15 persons who had vestibular rehabilitation reported improvement, but the natural history of MDDs is to improve, and one wonders what would have happened had they not undergone rehabilitation. In other words, this was an uncontrolled study, which sheds no light on whether PT is helpful. Cha commented in passing that "only rare patients seem to be cured by vestibular therapy" (Cha 2012). In fact, the only peer reviewed literature describing physical therapy treatment for MDDs are two case reports (Zimbelman and Watson 1992; Liphart et al, 2014). Of course, it is not known how these cases would have done without PT. In other words, these were not controlled. In general, while many individuals with MDD undergo vestibular rehabilitation, again because of a lack of controls, it is not possible to determine whether they did any better than persons who were not treated (Hain, Hanna et al. 1999). Thus the efficacy of vestibular rehabilitation for MDDs is unknown.

Motion sickness has been treated successfully with habituation (Dai, Raphan et al. 2011), and one might reasonably argue that MDD, being a motion sickness variant, might also respond to a similar approach. Habituation entails a down-weighting of motion input, and can reduce the long duration vestibular responses commonly associated with motion sickness susceptibility (Dai, Raphan et al. 2007). Although there are well developed self-directed motion habituation protocols such as the PUMA exercises (Puma 2010), there are presently no published reports of their efficacy in MDDs (or motion sickness for that matter). Nevertheless, we are sympathetic to the general idea that things that make you feel worse (when you are dizzy) usually does result in some improvement (if you can stand it). The Puma protocol exercises are just so extremely stimulating that so far -- nobody has been able to tolerate them for more than a session or two. The Puma protocol exercises can be bought on the web in the form of a DVD from Dr. Puma's website.

The roll adaptation protocol of Dai et al (2014) could be reasonably viewed as a type of habituation. We offer this treatment in our practice in Chicago. It is time intensive and we think only suitable for severe cases.

While we find this idea very doubtful, if MDD is indeed due to inappropriately high weighting of somatosensory input, vestibular physical therapy protocols that teach down-regulation of somatosensory input seem worth trying. Liphart (2015) reported results of "sensory reweighting therapy in a single atypical case. The single subject "felt she had improved 50%". This is not too different from results of roll adaptation discussed above. A controlled trial of vestibular rehabilitation in a large number (i.e. 20) of MDD subjects could be helpful in clearing up this question (hope someone funds this !). Our guess is that results of vestibular rehabilitation treatment would not be any different than no treatment.

If MDD is instead caused by an internal oscillator developed to predict boat motion, one's treatment strategy should be aimed at manipulation of psychological variables rather than somatosensory integration. Patients need to ignore their aberrant internal signal, in the same way that most persons with tinnitus eventually develop an ability to ignore abnormal internally generated sounds. Treatments that decrease vigilance, obsessiveness, and anxiety as well as "tincture of time", would be the optimum strategy.

Our suggestions for physical activity:

Going on another boat and related activities:

Research is needed !

MDD is not very well studied. As of 2013, only 29 papers came up on a "Pubmed" search for "mal de debarquement". Not much has changed in 2015 -- we now have 52 references with "debarquement" in our reference database. Considering that many other obscure conditions have 1000's of papers written about them, this means that MDD has been generally ignored.

There are many open questions. Here are a few:

We know of three ongoing research projects regarding MDD, two treatment projects in Oklahoma (Dr. Cha), another in Ohio(Clark)

We do not know of any treatment projects involving medication.

Related Links:

There is a MdDS foundation, which maintains it's own website and encompasses a quite active group of volunteers. The author of this page, Dr. Hain, is loosely associated -- I sometimes provide some advice to this group. There are also several other groups world-wide.

References on Mal de Debarquement Syndrome

Note that many of the references regarding MdDS are from lesser sources -- "open access" journals, review articles, articles for the public. Not much data here.

  1. anonymous. Classification and diagnostic criteria for headache disorders, cranial neuralgias and facial pain. Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Society. Cephalalgia. 1988;8 Suppl 7:1-96.
  2. Barmack, N.H. and H. Shojaku, Vestibularly induced slow oscillations in climbing fiber responses of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar nodulus of the rabbit. Neuroscience, 1992. 50(1): p. 1-5.
  3. Blakemore, S. J., et al. (1998). "Predicting the consequences of our own actions: the role of sensorimotor context estimation." J Neurosci 18(18): 7511-7518.
  4. Brown JJ, Baloh RW. Persistent mal de debarquement syndrome: a motion-induced subjective disorder of balance. American Journal of Otolaryngology. 1987;8:219-22.
  5. Cha, Y. H., J. Brodsky, et al. (2008). "Clinical features and associated syndromes of mal de debarquement." J Neurol 255(7): 1038-44.
  6. Cha, Y. H. (2009). "Mal de debarquement." Semin Neurol 29(5): 520-527. (This is a collection of review articles).
  7. Cha, Y.H., Less common neuro-otologic disorders. Continuum (Minneap Minn), 2012. 18(5 Neuro-otology): p. 1142-57. (Continuum is a collection of review articles, and is not original research).
  8. Cha YH, Cui Y, Baloh RW. [ABSTRACT] Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation for Mal de Debarquement Syndrome., Neurotology. 2012 Nov 29.
  9. Cha, Y.H., Y. Cui, and R.W. Baloh, Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for mal de debarquement syndrome. Otol Neurotol, 2013. 34(1): p. 175-9. 25.
  10. Cha Y, Chakrapani S, Craig A, Baloh RW. Metabolic and Functional Connectivity Changes in Mal de Debarquement Syndrome PLOS, 2012 (This is an open access journal)
  11. Cha, Y. H. and S. Chakrapani (2015). "Voxel Based Morphometry Alterations in Mal de Debarquement Syndrome." PLoS One 10(8): e0135021. (This is an open access journal)
  12. Cohen H. Vertigo after sailing a nineteenth century ship. Journal of Vestibular Research. 1996;6:31-5.
  13. Cohen, B., Dai, M., Smouha, E., & Cho, C. (2015). Mal de debarquement syndrome. Neurology: Clinical practice, 369-370.
  14. Dai, M., T. Raphan, and B. Cohen, Labyrinthine lesions and motion sickness susceptibility. Exp Brain Res, 2007. 178(4): p. 477-87.
  15. Dai M, Rapan T, Cohen B. Adaptation of the angular vestibulo-ocular reflex to head movements in rotating frames of reference. Exp Br. res (2009)
  16. Dai, M., T. Raphan, and B. Cohen, Prolonged reduction of motion sickness sensitivity by visual-vestibular interaction. Exp Brain Res, 2011. 210(3-4): p. 503-13.
  17. Dai M, Cohen B, Smouha E, Cho C. Readaptation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex relieves the mal de debarquement syndrome. Front Neurol 15, 2014. (This is an open access journal).
  18. Darwin E. Zoonomia, Vol. I. Or, the Laws of Organic Life. See http://www.gutenberg.org to download this book.
  19. DeFlorio, P. T. and R. Silbergleit (2006). "Mal de debarquement presenting in the Emergency Department." J Emerg Med 31(4): 377-9.
  20. Gibbs, C. R., et al. (2010). "'Sea legs': sharpened Romberg test after three days on a live-aboard dive boat." Diving Hyperb Med 40(4): 189-194.
  21. Gordon CR, Spitzer O, Shupak A, Doweck I. Survey of mal de debarquement. BMJ. 1992;304:544.
  22. Gordon CR, Spitzer O, Doweck I, Melamed Y, Shupak A. Clinical features of mal de debarquement: adaptation and habituation to sea conditions. Journal of Vestibular Research. 1995;5:363-9.
  23. Gordon, C.R., A. Shupak, and Z. Nachum, Mal de debarquement. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg, 2000. 126(6): p. 805-6.
  24. Graybiel A. Structural elements in the concept of motion sickness. Aerospace Medicine. 1969;40:351-67.
  25. Guofa, S., et al., Changes of symptom and EEG in mal de debarquement syndrome patients after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over bilateral prefrontal cortex: a pilot study. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc, 2014. 2014: p. 4294-7. (This is not a journal -- this is just a conference proceeding -- generally these are unreviewed)
  26. Hain TC, Hanna PA, Rheinberger MA. Mal de Debarquement. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 1999;125:615-620
  27. Hain TC. Mal de Debarquement. Hearing Health, 2005 (this article is intended for the public -- it is not peer reviewed).
  28. Hain TC, Yacovino D. Mal de Debarquement. In Geriatric Otolaryngology (Calhoun KH, Eibling DE Eds) Marcel Dekker, NY, NY. 2006, 125-133
  29. Hain TC , Helminski JO. Mal de Debarquement. in "Vestibular Rehabilitation", 2nd edn (Ed. S. Herdman), 2007
  30. Hain TC, H. J. (2014). Mal de Debarquement. Vestibular Rehabilitation. 4th edn. (Ed S. Herdman).
  31. Jacobson GP, Newman CW. The development of the Dizziness Handicap Inventory. Archives of Otolaryngology -- Head & Neck Surgery. 1990;116:424-7.
  32. Kayan A, Hood JD. Neuro-otological manifestations of migraine. Brain. 1984;107:1123-42.
  33. Kleinschmidt HJ, Collewijn H. A search for habituation of vestibulo-ocular reactions to rotatory and linear sinusoidal accelerations in the rabbit. Experimental Neurology. 1975;47:257-67.
  34. Liphart, J., Use of Sensory Reweighting for a Woman With Persistent Mal de Debarquement: A Case Report. J Geriatr Phys Ther, 2014.
  35. Liphart, J. (2015). "Use of sensory reweighting for a woman with persistent mal de debarquement: a case report." J Geriatr Phys Ther 38(2): 96-103.
  36. Mair IWS. The mal de debarquement syndrome. Journal of Audiological Medicine. 1996;5:21-25.
  37. Moeller L, Lempert T. Mal de debarquement: Pseudo-hallucinations from vestibular memory? J Neurol 2007.
  38. Murphy TP. Mal de debarquement syndrome: a forgotten entity? Otolaryngology - Head & Neck Surgery. 1993;109:10-3.
  39. Murphy S. Rare malady strikes cruising women over forty. Houston Women's Health Examiner (newspaper article) August 25, 10:59 PM
  40. Nachum, Z., et al., Mal de debarquement and posture: reduced reliance on vestibular and visual cues. Laryngoscope, 2004. 114(3): p. 581-6. (note these authors define MdDS differently than most medical studies).
  41. Pearce, A. J., et al. (2015). "Efficacy of neurostimulation to treat symptoms of Mal de Debarquement Syndrome. A preliminary study using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation." J Neuropsychol 9(2): 336-341.
  42. Puma, S. Puma method for prevention of motion sickness. 2010 1/12/2013; Available from: http://www.pumamethod.com/index.php.
  43. Saha KC, Fife TD. Mal de debarquement syndrome: review and proposed diagnostic criteria. Neurol Clin Pract 2014: 5: 209-215 (this journal is not indexed in Pubmed, and peer review process may not be very strong).
  44. Smith PF, Darlington CL. A possible explanation for dizziness following SSRI discontinuation. Acta Oto-laryngologica, 2010.
  45. Stewart WF, Schecter A, Rasmussen BK. Migraine prevalence A review of population based studies. Neurology. 1994;44:S17-S23.
  46. Stewart WF, Lipton RB. Migraine headache: epidemiology and health care utilization. Cephalalgia. 1993;Suppl 12:41-6.
  47. Stoffregen, T. A., et al. (2013). "Getting Your Sea Legs." PLoS One 8(6): e66949. (comment: This study is of landsickness, not of MDD; it is an open access journal as well)
  48. Stott JR. Adaptation to nauseogenic motion stimuli and its application in the treatment of airsickness. In: Crampton GH, ed. Motion and Space Sickness: CRC Press; 1990.
  49. Teitelbaum P. Mal de debarquement syndrome: a case report.. J Travel Med. 9(1): 51-2; 2002
  50. Van Ombergen, A., Van Rompaey, V., Maes, L. K., Van de Heyning, P. H., & Wuyts, F. L. (2015). Mal de debarquement syndrome: a systematic review. J Neurol. doi: 10.1007/s00415-015-7962-6
  51. Wassmer, E., P. Davies, et al. (2003). "Clinical spectrum associated with cerebellar hypoplasia." Pediatr Neurol 28(5): 347-351.
  52. Wolpert, D. M., et al. (1995). "An internal model for sensorimotor integration." Science 269(5232): 1880-1882.
  53. Yacovino, D. A. and F. J. Gualtieri (2006). "[Mal de debarquement syndrome in modern life]." Rev Neurol 43(9): 568-70.
  54. Zimbelman JL, Walton TM. Vestibular rehabilitation of a patient with persistent Mal de Debarquement. Physical Therapy Case Reports, 1999;2(4):129-137

Acknowledgement:

Click here to return to index

Copyright June 15, 2016 , Timothy C. Hain, M.D. All rights reserved. Last saved on June 15, 2016